Tuesday 26 November 2013

Musical Training has Long-Lasting Effects



The other day, as I was dropping off my son at school, I was approached by another mom, who asked for my business card and said she was thinking about putting her 6-year-old daughter in piano lessons.  She told me that she’d been reading a lot in the news about how studying music was good for brain development.

I’ve noticed this too:  a flurry of news articles in the last few weeks extolling the benefits of musical training for children’s brains.  Music training altering the brain isn’t really news, but here's what is: several recent research studies show that a relatively short period of musical training early in life can have long-lasting effects on the brain.

Nina Kraus’ lab at Northwestern University has published a stack of papers showing that musical training alters the way the brain processes sound.  One of the ways they study auditory processing is by looking at the electrical responses of the brainstem in response to sounds such as speech.  One particular type of sound in speech that can be difficult to process is the transition between consonants and vowels.  Because this transition happens so quickly, it can be hard to tell apart the sound "da" from the sound "ba", for example.  A fast brainstem response to consonant-vowel transitions indicates that this type of auditory input is well-processed, and this in turn leads to enhanced language abilities.  Kraus’ lab has shown in the past that musicians’ brains have faster responses to these types of transitions than the brains of non-musicians.

A new study from Kraus’ lab, published this month in the Journal of Neuroscience, compares brainstem responses to consonant-vowel transitions in older adults who either had no musical training, a small amount of musical training (1-3 years) as a child, or a moderate amount of musical training (4-14 years) as a child.  Note that all of the musical training had occurred while the subjects were children, and so it had been roughly 50 years since these people had had music lessons.

Despite the fact that it had been so long since musical training ended, there were noticeable differences in the responses to consonant-vowel transitions in the brains of the three different groups.  The fastest responses were from the people with the most musical training, while the people with no musical training had the slowest responses.

From White-Schwoch et al. (2013).  Group average brainstem responses to the sound “da”.  The blue lines (fastest response) are the subjects with moderate musical training, orange is little musical training, and grey lines are the responses from people with no musical training.



This study shows that the effects of musical training can persist long after the musical training has stopped.  Why would this be?  We tend to think of our brain capabilities in a “use it or lose it” fashion – for example, if you don’t play the piano for 50 years, you’re probably not going to be able to play it very well.  But in this case, what the musical training did was to alter the way the brain processes sound, and since processing sound is something that we do in everyday life, the enhanced neural responses to sound continued to be used after the end of musical training.  The authors of this study suggest that early musical training primes the brain’s auditory system for further plasticity in response to sound, setting the stage for future auditory effects on the brain.

Another study, presented at the recent annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, showed that people who had musical training that started before the age of seven had thicker gray matter in certain areas of the brain than those who started musical training later.  This research, authored by Yunxin Wang at Beijing Normal University, indicates that starting lessons before the age of seven is more likely to lead to long-lasting brain changes, something that has been suggested by previous research.  What is slightly different about Wang’s study, though, is that the subjects were not all currently musicians; some had had only a small amount of musical training in childhood and not continued with lessons.  Nevertheless, that early musical training seems to have left an impression in their brains.  Whether the differences in brain anatomy lead to any change in brain function is not yet known, but this type of result certainly suggests that starting music lessons earlier is more likely to lead to long-lasting neurological benefits.


References

Wang, Y., Xei, L., Zhu, B., Liu, Q., and Dong, Q. (2013). It matters when you start: The age of onset of music training predicts brain anatomy.  Program No. 765.07, Neuroscience 2013 Abstracts, San Diego, CA: Society for Neuroscience, Online.

White-Schwoch, T., Carr, K.W., Anderson, S., Strait, D.L., and Kraus, N. (2013). Older adults benefit from music training early in life: biological evidence for long-term training-driven plasticity. J. Neurosci. 33, 17667–17674.

No comments:

Post a Comment